Abstract
Human thinking is modeled on the body in much the same way as hominid thinking was (Sheets-Johnstone, 1990, 4, 25). The hominid experience of moving was the cornerstone of the experience of the precinct of I can. The experience of I can subsequently formed the basis for corporeal and topological concepts. These, in turn, generated analogical insights. One sees, accordingly, not only an intermingling of the bodily experience and discernment, but also everywhere indications of analogical thinking at the root of hominid tool-building. Self-movement is the mainspring of one’s experience of oneself as an agent endowed with free-will-related activities. It is the creative fount of one’s ideas of space and time. The phenomenon of moving oneself structures how one knows the world. Moving oneself is a manner of coming to knowledge. The Lives of species-specific bodies are built on thinking in movement. Studies provide ample evidence to show a fundamental correlation between motor and cognitive development. This developmental correlation is also found in both the prefrontal cortex and the cerebellum. Both cognitive and motor developments reveal a prolonged developmental timetable. Both the prefrontal cortex and the cerebellum attain maturity much later. Studies by Adele Diamond and others show that the cerebellum is not only involved in motor operations but also in cognitive functions. Conversely, the prefrontal cortex, with (the help of) its connection with the cortical and subcortical areas that are critical for movement control, is involved in both motor and cognitive functions. Thus both motor and cognitive behaviours seem to belong together; and the body appears crucially involved in the activities of the mind (Diamond, 2000, 44, 49; Bushnell & Boudreau, 1993, 1008, 1015-1017; Burns et al., 2004, 19-29)References:
1) Bartlett, D. & Piper, M. C.,1993; Neuromotor Development of Preterm Infants Through the First Year of Life. PMID: 12(4):37-55.
2) Branco, T. & Redgrave, P., July 2020, The Neural Basis of Escape Behavior in Vertebrates, Annual Review of Neuroscience, Vol. 43:417-439 (https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-100219-122527)
3) Burns, Y., O’Callaghan, M, et al., 2004, Movement and Motor Development in ELBW Infants at 1 Year is Related to Cognitive and Motor Abilities at 4 Years, Early Human Development, 80: 1, pp. 19-29.
4) Bushnell, E.W., Boudreau, J.P., 1993, Motor Development and the Mind, The Potential Role of Motor Abilities as a Determinant of Aspects of Perceptual Development. Child Development, 64, 1005-1021.
5) Carpenter, C.R., 1963, Societies of Monkeys and Apes, in Charles H. Southwick (ed.), Primate Social Behaviour, New York, Van Nostrand.
6) Diamond, A., 2000, Close Interrelation of Motor Development and Cognitive Development and of the Cerebellum and Prefrontal Cortex, Child Development 71:1, pp. 44-56.
7) Edelman, Gerald M., 1992, Bright Air, Brilliant Fire: On the Matter of the Mind, Basic Books, New York, NY.
8) Evans, S. M. (Dec. 1971), Behavior in Polychaetes, The Quarterly Review of Biology, Vol. 46, No. 4, pp. 379-405.
9) Flament, D., Ellermann, J., Ugurbil, K., & Ebner, T.J., 1994, Functional Gagnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) of Cerebellar Activation while Learning to Correct for Visuomotor Errors, Society for Neuroscience, Abstracts, 20:20.
10) Gilbert, P. F. & W T Thach, W. T., 1977 June, Purkinje cell activity during motor learning, Brain Res .10;128(2):309-28.
11) Goyen T.C., Lui K., 2002, Longitudinal Motor Development of Apparently Normal High-risk Infants at 18 Months, 3 and 5 Years, Early Human development 70, pp. 103-116.
12) Goyen T.C., Lui K., Woods, R., 1998 Feb. Visual-motor, visual-perceptual, and fine motor outcomes in very-low-
birthweight children at 5 years. Dev Med Child Neurol 40(2):76-81.
13) Hepper, P., 10 February 2015, Behavior During the Prenatal Period: Adaptive for Development and Survival. Child
Development Perspectives Volume 9, Issue 1 p. 38-43
14) Ivry, Richard, June 1993, Cerebellar Involvement in the Explicit Representation of Temporal Information a. Annals of
the New York Academy of Sciences, Volume 682, Issue 1, pp. 214-230
15) Klatzky, R.L., Lederman, S.J., & Metzger, V. (1985). Identifying objects by touch: An "expert system". Perception &
Psychophysics, 37 (4), 299-302.
16) Merleau-Ponty, M., 1962, Phenomenology of Perception, Colin Smith (trans.), London, Routledge.
17) Merleau-Ponty, M., 1968, The Visible and the Invisible, Claude Lefort (ed.), Alphonso Lingis (trans.), Evanston,
Northwestern University Press.
18) Sheets-Johnstone, M., 1999, The Primacy of Movement, Philadelphia, John Benjamins.
19) Sheets-Johnstone, M., 1990, The Roots of Thinking, Philadelphia, Temple University Press.
20) Tarkan, L., 2004, Tracking Stress and Depression Back to the Womb. New York Times, Dec.7.
21) Tinbergen, N. (1968). On war and peace in animals and man. Science, 160(3835), 1411–1418
22) Worldwildlife, 2023, 5 Remarkable Animal Dads (https://www.worldwildlife.org/stories/5-remarkable-animal-dads)
23) Earth Reminder, January 9, 2023, 17 Animals That Protect Their Family and Young Ones
(https://www.earthreminder.com/animals-that-protect-their-family-and-young/)